Water Scarcity Changes How People Think (2024)

Our brain is sensitive to scarcity. The lack of something we consider vital, such as time or food, can powerfully shape our thinking and behavior. Take money, for example. When people play a game that makes some players abruptly wealthier or poorer, those who lose money start making decisions that result in them being better off now but worse off later.

One potential explanation for why this happens is that scarcity is mentally taxing: the sense of not having enough becomes a sort of distraction that makes it harder to focus and plan.

In recent work, however, we found that people react to water scarcity very differently than they do to other shortages. In studies of people around the world, we found that those in places where water is scarce think more about the long term. And confronting people with water scarcity in the lab made them focus on the future. Worrying about water shortages prompted better planning and less wasteful behavior—not only with water but with other resources, too.

As a species that is acutely dependent on water—without it, we would die within days—we seem to be wired to plan for it. The loss of water prompts a general mindset of thrift and long-term thinking. This discovery could have important implications for humankind’s response to climate change.

[Read more about humanity’s reliance on water]

We explored thinking about water in several ways. In one experiment, we brought 211 college students to the lab and asked a portion of them to read an article we provided. Some received an article that was dire. It detailed predictions of the worst “water shortage in 1,200 years.” Others read an article that was more upbeat. It described how climate change means more rain and therefore ample water. (The latter article focused on the positives of water availability and did not, for example, discuss threat of floods.) The rest of the students completed the study without reading an article.

We then asked the students how important it was to save resources and think about the long term. We found that the mere idea of water scarcity was enough to kick-start them into thinking about the future. Participants who read about a serious shortage agreed more with survey items such as “People should live for the future.” They also agreed more with items about saving, such as “There are things I resist buying today so I can save for tomorrow.”

In contrast, reading about a future water surplus pushed people in the opposite direction. They endorsed living for the present and cared less about saving money and other resources, compared with those who had answered the questions without reading about climate change at all.

Of course, simply reading about water scarcity might have different effects than actually experiencing it. So we looked for places where communities have experienced real water scarcity for generations. We found two geographically close cities in Iran—Shiraz and Yazd—that have similar economies and the same majority religion, language and ethnicity but sharply different amounts of water. Shiraz receives enough rainfall to support vineyards that produce its world-famous wine. Nearby Yazd is bone-dry. We gave psychological tests to 331 people in Shiraz and Yazd to measure what psychologists call “long-term orientation,” or how much individuals prioritize the future. Sure enough, people in Yazd thought that planning for the future was more important than those in Shiraz—while people in Shiraz liked the idea of living in the moment more than those in Yazd.

Then we went a step further. Iran is particularly vulnerable to drought, so perhaps people there are more sensitive to water scarcity than populations in other places. To get some sense of whether similar patterns exist elsewhere, we turned to the World Values Survey, a long-running global research project that gathers information about people’s beliefs and values. We focused on survey data that asked respondents in 87 countries about the importance of thrift and saving for the future. We found that those who live in countries with a history of water scarcity tended to agree more with saving for the future. For example, in Europe, people in water-rich Iceland thought less about the future, whereas those in dry Spain thought about the future more.

Importantly, countries’ history of water scarcity explained cultural differences beyond other, more obvious factors. For example, income per capita did not explain differences across cultures. Although corruption might make it hard for people to think about the future, it was not a strong predictor either. You might guess that people think about the future more in places where they tend to live longer, but astonishingly, national life expectancy was not as strong of a predictor as water scarcity.

Across studies, our findings suggest that water has a powerful place in our thinking—one that’s distinct from other important resources, including wealth. In fact, humans may have evolved to be acutely sensitive to water scarcity because it’s so critical to us. Humans managed without money for many thousands of years. Our species can endure without food for weeks but without water for just days.

There is some evidence of an evolutionary wiring for water if we look at our sense of smell. Mice have about 1,000 functional genes that encode smell receptors, whereas humans have only 400 (plus around 600 genes that may no longer be functional). Yet humans are better at detecting the scent of fresh rainfall than sharks are at detecting blood. The fact that water is so critical for human life makes it more plausible that evolution would wire us to have psychological reactions that are specific to the threat of water scarcity.

That sensitivity may be crucial going forward. Climate change is making droughts more common. Our work suggests that as many places dry up, global warming could reshape how people think, pushing whole communities toward more cautious, future-oriented behavior. That may offer a sliver of hope amid the threat of climate change. Water scarcity could be a uniquely powerful motivator to prepare for and respond to a warmer world.

Are you a scientist who specializes in neuroscience, cognitive science or psychology? And have you read a recent peer-reviewed paper that you would like to write about for Mind Matters? Please send suggestions to Scientific American’sMind Matters editor Daisy Yuhasat dyuhas@sciam.com.

This is an opinion and analysis article, and the views expressed by the author or authors are not necessarily those ofScientific American.

I am an expert in cognitive science and psychology, with a deep understanding of the human brain's sensitivity to scarcity and its impact on decision-making and behavior.

The article discusses the impact of scarcity, particularly water scarcity, on human thinking and behavior. It highlights how scarcity of vital resources, such as water, can shape decision-making and planning. The author presents findings from studies that demonstrate how people react to water scarcity differently than they do to other shortages. The article also discusses the implications of these findings for humankind's response to climate change.

Scarcity's Impact on Decision-Making and Behavior

The lack of something vital, such as time, food, or money, can powerfully shape our thinking and behavior. When people experience scarcity, it can lead to decisions that prioritize immediate gains but result in long-term disadvantages .

Water Scarcity and Long-Term Thinking

Contrary to the impact of other shortages, studies show that people react to water scarcity by thinking more about the long term. Individuals in places where water is scarce tend to focus on the future and engage in better planning and less wasteful behavior, not only with water but with other resources as well.

Impact of Reading about Water Scarcity

Reading about water scarcity prompts individuals to think more about the future and prioritize saving resources. Conversely, reading about a future water surplus pushes people in the opposite direction, leading them to endorse living for the present and care less about saving money and resources.

Real-Life Impact of Water Scarcity

Real-life experiences of water scarcity in communities have also been studied. In geographically close cities in Iran, Shiraz and Yazd, with differing water availability, psychological tests revealed that people in the water-scarce city prioritized planning for the future more than those in the water-rich city.

Global Patterns of Water Scarcity

The impact of water scarcity extends globally. Survey data from 87 countries showed that those living in countries with a history of water scarcity tended to agree more with saving for the future. This pattern was observed beyond other factors such as income per capita, corruption, and national life expectancy.

Evolutionary Sensitivity to Water Scarcity

The article also discusses the evolutionary wiring for water scarcity, highlighting the critical role of water for human life. It suggests that humans may have evolved to be acutely sensitive to water scarcity, leading to specific psychological reactions to the threat of water scarcity.

In conclusion, the article provides compelling evidence of the unique impact of water scarcity on human thinking and behavior, with potential implications for addressing climate change.

Water Scarcity Changes How People Think (2024)
Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Moshe Kshlerin

Last Updated:

Views: 6164

Rating: 4.7 / 5 (77 voted)

Reviews: 92% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Moshe Kshlerin

Birthday: 1994-01-25

Address: Suite 609 315 Lupita Unions, Ronnieburgh, MI 62697

Phone: +2424755286529

Job: District Education Designer

Hobby: Yoga, Gunsmithing, Singing, 3D printing, Nordic skating, Soapmaking, Juggling

Introduction: My name is Moshe Kshlerin, I am a gleaming, attractive, outstanding, pleasant, delightful, outstanding, famous person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.